Автор работы: n**********@mail.ru, 27 Ноября 2011 в 12:33, реферат
In its more elaborate form componental analysis also proceeds from the assumption that word-meaning is not an unanalysable whole but can be decomposed into elementary semantic components. it is assumed that any item can be described in terms of categories arranged in a hierarchical way; that is a subsequent category is a subcategory of the previous category.
In this analysis linguists proceed from the assumption that the smallest units of meaning are semes) and that sememes and lexemes are usually not in one-to-one but in one-to-many correspondence.
For example, in the lexical item woman several components of meaning or semes may be singled out and namely ‘human’, ‘female’, ‘adult’
The analysis of the word girl would also yield the sememes ‘human’ and ‘female’, but instead of the sememe ‘adult’ we shall find the sememe ‘young’ distinguishing the meaning of the word woman from that of girl.
The comparison of the results of the componental analysis of the words boy and girl would also show the difference just in one component, i..e. the sememe denoting ‘male’ and ‘female’ respectively.
In its more elaborate form componental analysis also proceeds from the assumption that word-meaning is not an unanalysable whole but can be decomposed into elementary semantic components. it is assumed that any item can be described in terms of categories arranged in a hierarchical way; that is a subsequent category is a subcategory of the previous category.
The most inclusive categories are parts of speech — the major word classes are nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs. All members of a major class share a distinguishing semantic feature and involve a certain type of semantic information. More revealing names for such features might be “thingness” or “substantiality” for nouns, “quality” for adjectives, and so on.
All other
semantic features may be classified into semantic markers — semantic
features which are present also in the lexical meaning of other words
and distinguishers — semantic features which are individual, i.e.
which do not recur in the lexical meaning of other words.. The
componental analysis of the word, e.g., spinster runs: noun, count-noun,
human, adult, female, who has never married. Noun
of course is the part of speech, meaning the most inclusive category;
count-noun is a marker, it represents a subclass within nouns and
refers to the semantic feature which the word spinster has in common
with all other countable nouns (boy, table, flower, idea, etc.) but
which distinguishes it from all uncountable nouns, e.g. salt, bread,
water, etc; human is also a marker which refers the word spinster
to a subcategory of countable nouns, i.e. to nouns denoting human beings;
adult is another marker pointing at a specific subdivision of human
beings into adults & young or not grown up. The word spinster possesses
still another marker — female — which it shares with such
words as woman, widow, mother, etc., and which represents a subclass
of adult females. At last comes the distinguisher who has never married
which differentiates the meaning of the word from other words which
have all other common semantic features.
The theory of Immediate Constituents (IC) was originally elaborated as an attempt to determine the ways in which lexical units are relevantly related to one another. It was discovered that combinations of such units are usually structured into hierarchically arranged sets of binary constructions. For example in the word-group a black dress in severe style we do not relate a to black, black to dress, dress to in, etc. but set up a structure which may be represented as a black dress / in severe style. Thus the fundamental aim of IC analysis is to segment a set of lexical units into two maximally independent sequences or ICs thus revealing the hierarchical structure of this set. Successive segmentation results in Ultimate Constituents (UC), i.e. two-facet units that cannot be segmented into smaller units having both sound-form and meaning. The Ultimate Constituents of the word-group analysed above are: a | black | dress | in | severe | style.
The meaning of the sentence, word-group, etc. and the IC binary segmentation are interdependent. For example, fat major’s wife may mean that either ‘the major is fat’ or ‘his wife is fat’. The former semantic interpretation presupposes the IC analysis into fat major’s | wife, whereas the latter reflects a different segmentation into IC’s and namely fat | major’s wife.
It must be admitted that this kind of analysis is arrived at by reference to intuition and it should be regarded as an attempt to formalise one’s semantic intuition.
It is mainly to discover the derivational structure of words that IC analysis is used in lexicological investigations. For example, the verb denationalise has both a prefix de- and a suffix -ise (-ize). To decide whether this word is a prefixal or a suffixal derivative we must apply IC analysis.1 The binary segmentation of the string of morphemes making up the word shows that *denation or *denational cannot be considered independent sequences as there is no direct link between the prefix de- and nation or national. In fact no such sound-forms function as independent units in modern English. The only possible binary segmentation is de | nationalise, therefore we may conclude that the word is a prefixal derivative. There are also numerous cases when identical morphemic structure of different words is insufficient proof of the identical pattern of their derivative structure which can be revealed only by IC analysis. Thus, comparing, e.g., snow-covered and blue-eyed we observe that both words contain two root-morphemes and one derivational morpheme. IC analysis, however, shows that whereas snow-covered may be treated as a compound consisting of two stems snow + covered, blue-eyed is a suffixal derivative as the underlying structure as shown by IC analysis is different, i.e. (blue+eye)+-ed.
It may be inferred from the examples discussed above that ICs represent the word-formation structure while the UCs show the morphemic structure of polymorphic words.
By the term distributionthe
position which lexical units occupy or may occupy in the text or in
the flow of speech. It is readily observed that a certain component
of the word-meaning is described when the word is identified distributionally.
For example, in the sentence The boy
— home the missing word is easily identified as a verb — The
boy went, came, ran, etc. home. Thus, we see that the component of meaning
that is distributionally identified is actually the part-of-speech meaning
but not the individual lexical meaning of the word under analysis.
It is also observed
that in a number of cases words have different lexical meanings in different
distributional patterns. Compare, e.g., the lexical meaning of the verb
to treat in the following: to treat somebody well, kindly, etc. —
‘to act or behave towards’ where the verb is followed by a noun
+ an adverb and to treat somebody to ice-cream, champagne, etc.
— ‘to supply with food, drink, entertainment, etc. at one’s own
expence’ where the verb is followed by a noun+the preposition
to + another noun
Distribution of stems in a compound makes part of the lexical meaning of the compound word. Compare, e.g., different lexical meanings of the words formed by the same stems bird and cage in bird-cage and cage-bird.
It is also assumed that productivity largely depends on the distributional meaning of the lexical units. Distributional meaning of the lexical units accounts for the possibility of making up and understanding a lexical item that has never been heard or used before but whose distributional pattern is familiar to the speaker and the hearer. Thus, though such words as kissable, hypermagical, smiler (She is a charming smiler), etc. cannot be found in any dictionary their meaning is easily understood on the analogy with other words having the same distributional pattern, e. g- (v + -able- -> A as in readable, eatable and kissable).
Информация о работе The most inclusive categories are parts of speech